by Theodore Dalrymple (July 2018)
Figures in Smoke #2, William Baziotes, 1947
Whenever a book is described or promoted as a best-seller, I tend not to buy or to read it. Exodus tells us that ‘Thou shalt not follow a multitude to do evil,’ which I have adapted slightly in my mind to ‘Thou shalt not follow a multitude to read rubbish.’ I pass best-sellers by.
However, my buying and reading habits represent a tendency rather than an invariable rule in the manner of a law of the universe; and there is no book that is not instinctive in one way or another. To know what a multitude reads or watches is important to understanding how and perhaps even why it thinks and feels as it does: though in many cases best-sellers, at least of non-fiction, reflect what multitudes already think or feel. Most people, after all, read to confirm what they already know or suspect rather than to learn something new or extend their horizons. We need more often to be reminded than informed, said Dr Johnson, to which he might have added, we desire more often to have our views confirmed than to lay ourselves open to the painful necessity of changing our minds about anything important. By comparison with changing one’s mind on any important matter, moving house, the pains of which I am the last to minimise, is but a minor inconvenience.
I decided from curiosity to buy The Dangerous Case of Donald Trump: 27 Psychiatrists and Mental Health Experts Assess a President, edited by Dr Bandy Lee, a Yale psychiatrist. It is, or was, a best-seller, though not, I imagine, among supporters of or sympathisers with the President. I suspected, correctly, that it would principally be an echo-chamber for the thoughts and feelings of those who already abominated him.
Most of the 27 were psychotherapists rather than psychiatrists, that is to say were not medically-qualified, and therefore were not bound by the so-called Goldwater Rule. This was established after the fiasco of a survey conducted among psychiatrists in 1964 about the psychological fitness of Senator Barry Goldwater, the Republican candidate for the presidency who ran against the incumbent, Lyndon B Johnson, in which 1,189 psychiatrists opined that, for purely psychological reason, Goldwater was unfit for the presidency. They were not asked to consider, nor did they express an opinion on, the psychological fitness for the presidency of Lyndon B Johnson, by all accounts a man of many psychological shortcomings. Indeed, the presidency might almost be defined as the job for which no one is psychologically fitted; going a little further, we could define life itself as that state or enterprise for which Mankind is unfitted.
It was perfectly obvious that the psychiatrists in 1964 were merely relaying their political preferences rather than providing the public with any independent or dispassionate knowledge. In the wake of the fiasco, the American Psychiatric Association, trying to repair the damage, elaborated the Goldwater Rule, making it illegal for a psychiatrist to express an opinion, qua psychiatrist (that is to say with such authority as psychiatry possesses), on the mental health of a public figure whom that psychiatrist has not examined in person, and without his or her permission.
I am not quite sure about the propriety of this rule. It seems to be almost a condition of being a public figure these days that you must be prepared to forego your privacy. Major public figures have their lives sifted as gold prospectors pan for gold, in this case the gold being tiny flecks of scandal that (to change the metaphor) will serve as grist to the mill of his or her opponents and detractors. This is more so than ever before: a single Tweet can destroy a career, as a bad investment can destroy a fortune accumulated over many years. This, no doubt, is an unfortunate development, but it is difficult to see how, in modern conditions, it can be otherwise. A public figure must either be a saint—and even saints are not saintly until they have been retrospectively canonised—or pachydermatous, that is to say so ambitious that they do not mind being denigrated or traduced. In other words, anyone who seeks high office is thereby psychologically unsuited for it, now more than ever before in history. Only in those who have greatness thrust upon them is there any hope.
My main objection to the book was not so much its transgression of a rule of professional ethics, but its profound, though predictable, banality. The fact that it was written by 27 ‘mental health experts’ (whatever they might be) detracted rather than added to whatever authority such a volume might have had. One senses immediately, and in the event correctly, that one is in the presence of groupthink rather than of 27 minds that have independently come to the same or very similar conclusions. To read their contributions is a little like reading 27 copies of the same newspaper to try to establish whether one of its stories is true.
What the contributions do inadvertently expose is the banality of so much psychiatric or psychological diagnosis, which amounts to little more than re-description of easily and publicly observable traits and conduct. Anyone reading the 360 pages of this book will probably come away with nothing new to him, no fact that he did not know before and no opinion that he had not heard before.
The explanatory value of the diagnoses offered by the ‘experts’ is virtually nil. They say that Mr Trump has a personality disorder, that is to say exhibits certain undesirable traits of character that have remained, and will remain, constant over his lifetime, for example narcissism to an unusual degree. We know he had narcissistic personality disorder because he is narcissistic; he is narcissistic because he has narcissistic personality disorder. This is the kind of ‘explanation’ that Moliere ridiculed more than three centuries ago: opium produces sleep because it has within it a dormitive property. Nor do we need psychiatrists or psychologists or psychotherapists to tell us that people usually retain the characteristics that they have.
We are informed in the volume on many occasions that Mr Trump is as he is because he lacks fundamental self-esteem: that is why he is so narcissistic, he is always trying desperately to compensate for his permanently damaged self-conception. Of course, if he were a morbidly shy and retiring man the same lack of self-esteem would explain it. In effect, then, the same factor explains everything from the grossest exhibitionism to the most profound social withdrawal.
But why does Mr Trump have such low self-esteem that he must plaster himself all over television and the world’s press in order to feel truly valuable, though never quite managing it because the original wound is unhealable?
The answer lies in his father, a dominant and demanding man who was never satisfied with what his son did or achieved, and always wanted more of him. Moreover, Mr Trump’s father was a self-made man in a way that his son could never be. Not only could the young Trump never receive the unconditional or unreserved approval of his father, but he could never equal his achievement, however hard he tried: hence his permanent tendency to self-aggrandisement and also his lack of trust in others. They would always be thinking ‘He is not as good, important, clever, as he thinks he is, or as his father was.’
There is a kind of plausibility to this, but plausibility is a poor guide to explanatory power. It so happens that shortly after I read this book, I read also Anthony Storr’s long essay on Winston Churchill, titled Churchill’s Black Dog, first published in 1969.
Now Storr, a psychiatrist, was a graceful and urbane writer (which the contributors to the collective volume decidedly are not), a psychiatrist with a respectful but not obsequious attitude to ancestors such as Freud and Jung. Indeed, he was more respectful towards them than most psychiatrists would be inclined to be today, but when one reads him one senses that one is in the presence of a civilised, cultivated and intelligent mind.
How does he explain Churchill’s extraordinary career and multiple achievements? Why, lack of self-esteem, of course.
The argument goes like this. His parents were more or less absent from his childhood, having better things to do (as they thought) than to attend to a mere child such as he, whom they left first to his nanny and then to a series of boarding schools. He thus never experienced the unconditional love and concern of parents that give to children a fundamental sense of self-worth irrespective of what they do or become. A child who is not unconditionally loved, especially by parents who themselves are loved or admired from afar, as it were, is condemned to try to make himself lovable, as if great achievement will somehow gain the love that has not be granted to him as of right. Unfortunately, no achievement, however great, will ever succeed in this; at best, it will only breed admiration, and usually not from those from whom love is most sought. Thus, a person such as Churchill is driven to achieve more and more, though no achievement never brings him what he wants, and he ends up rather with a sense of the futility of existence. Vanity of vanity, saith the man with low self-esteem, all is vanity; and when the striving for achievement is either defeated or has to stop, he is inclined to sink into black despair.
This all sounds perfectly plausible, at least superficially so. In fact, Storr himself exhibited the pattern that he describes. He had a lonely childhood too, deprived of parental warmth; and he too strove for great distinction, never managing to stave off completely a fundamentally depressive temperament by his literary activity, which was of considerable worth, at least aesthetically.
No doubt in the history of great achievement or striving for fame many other examples could be found, which would seem to confirm the hypothesis. But this manner of proceeding is deeply flawed; it is a prime example of confirmation bias, the seeking and finding of new evidence to support a conclusion already arrived at.
I suppose that the correct way of proceeding would be to choose people remarkable for their achievement or at least their ambition, without knowing anything of their individual biographies, and then investigate whether they suffered distant or disapproving parenting by comparison with a group of controls who were not particularly high-achieving or ambitious, controlling also for as many relevant factors as possible, such as social class etc. This is the counsel of perfection, and in any such investigations such as this would lead only to statistical generalisations rather than to Newtonian-type laws: no one would expect that only those deprived of unconditional parental regard and love ever became unusually ambitious, for humanity is too various to be corralled into a few simple generalisations. In human affairs, exceptions do not prove the rule, they are the rule.
In the meantime, we are condemned to be guided by mere plausibility as the explanation of human conduct; and while Storr’s hypothesis is indeed plausible, so too would its opposite be: that someone who had enjoyed the unconditional love of his parents had the self-confidence to go out and conquer the world, taking failure in his stride. Ultimate failure would cause him to become depressed because it would confront him with the unjustifiability of the initial premise of his life, that he was a person of great worth. He would therefore maintain his efforts, notwithstanding any failures en route, in order precisely to avoid the disproof of the premise on which he has constructed his existence.
The ultimate vanity of ambition is almost a literary cliché. Gray tells us:
The boast of heraldry, the pomp of pow’r,
And all that beauty, all the wealth e’er gave,
Awaits alike th’inevitable hour.
The paths of glory lead but to the grave.
And Shelley:
And on the pedestal, these words appear:
My name is Ozymandias, King of Kings;
Look on my Works, ye Mighty, and despair!
But of course both Gray and Shelley had abusive, distant and disapproving fathers, which caused in them an inordinate desire to write immortal lines.
_____________________________
Theodore Dalrymple’s latest book is Grief and Other Stories from New English Review Press.
NER on Twitter @NERIconoclast
- Like
- Digg
- Del
- Tumblr
- VKontakte
- Buffer
- Love This
- Odnoklassniki
- Meneame
- Blogger
- Amazon
- Yahoo Mail
- Gmail
- AOL
- Newsvine
- HackerNews
- Evernote
- MySpace
- Mail.ru
- Viadeo
- Line
- Comments
- Yummly
- SMS
- Viber
- Telegram
- Subscribe
- Skype
- Facebook Messenger
- Kakao
- LiveJournal
- Yammer
- Edgar
- Fintel
- Mix
- Instapaper
- Copy Link